There is a lot of evidence in support of the social
identity explanation of in-group favoritism. However, my colleagues (Constantina Badea and Jolanda Jetten) and I have
recently questioned whether beating the out-group is the only motive for
in-group favoritism. We assumed that, in the specific case of low status
groups, in-group favoritism may be used to make the in-group equal to the out-group, and not
necessarily better than the out-group. In this case, in-group favoritism is used
to achieve intergroup fairness rather
than positive in-group distinctiveness. To give a real world example, gay men and women may favor their low
status group not because they want to outdo straight men and women but because they seek equality with straight people in having
the right to marry their partners. In this case, in-group favoritism is
enacted with the aim of achieving intergroup equality rather than high in-group status or positive distinctiveness.
An Equal Love Rally in Melbourne, Australia |
To clarify our assumptions, we distinguished between two
types of in-group favoritism that might be used by low status groups: competitive
favoritism and compensatory
favoritism.
- Low status groups can use competitive favoritism to compete with high status out-groups in order to achieve positive in-group distinctiveness.
- Low status groups can use compensatory favoritism to compensate for their low status and achieve intergroup fairness.
To test these predictions, we
asked 139 psychology undergraduates to take part in a social group resource
distribution task. Participants were given an identification number ranging
from 1 to 50 and then randomly assigned to two arbitrary groups called the “Red Group” and the “Green Group.”
They then allocated points to people from their group and the other group.
They were told to think of the points as "points in a game," where the more
points a person or group gets, the better. Crucially, the members of one group
always started this points allocation task with two more points than the members of the
other group. So, in the context of our admittedly artificial laboratory
situation, we had an intergroup status hierarchy, with a high status group starting
off with more points than a low status group.
Participants used a series of point allocation tables like
the one below to award points to group members. In each table, they had
to circle one of the four columns listed under the heading “My Allocation to
Each Person.” Some of the choices in these columns compensated the low status group member for their
initial two-point disadvantage. For example, if a member of the Red Group
circled the 3/5 choice in the table, then their fellow low status Red Group member would end up with the same
number of points (6 starting points + 5 allocated points = 11 points in total) as the high status Green Group member (8 starting points + 3 allocated points = 11 points in total). Other choices
in the tables allowed low status group members to get more points than the high
status group member. For example, the 2/6 choice meant that the Red group
member ended up with more points (6 + 6 = 12) than the Green group
member (8 + 2 = 10).
Using this novel approach, we found that participants in the low status group were significantly more likely than participants in the high status group to choose the competitive in-group favoritism option (e.g., the 2/6 choice in the above table). So, we replicated the classic, competitive favoritism effect that is predicted by social identity theory.
However, we also found that participants in the low status
group selected the compensatory favoritism option (e.g., the 3/5 choice in the table) significantly more than chance. Here, awarding more points to the in-group than to the out-group had the
effect of compensating the in-group for its initial points disadvantage and
leading to intergroup fairness. Hence,
the compensatory favoritism choice enabled the low status in-group to do as
well as the high status out-group in terms of its points allocations but not
necessarily better than the out-group.
Social identity theory has touted social competition as the key driver of social change (Tajfel and
Turner, 1979). Our research suggests that, in the context of unequal intergroup
status relations, social compensation can
also provided a pathway to social change. Specifically, low status
groups can use in-group favouritism to change the intergroup hierarchy by
getting even with the high status group, rather than by surpassing it in
status.
For further information, please see the following journal article:
Rubin, M., Badea, C., and Jetten, J. (2014). Low status groups show in-group favoritism to compensate for their low status and to compete for higher status. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations DOI: 10.1177/1368430213514122
For a self-archived version of this article, please see here.